By listening to this episode, you can earn 1.5 Psychiatry CME Credits.

Other Places to listen: iTunes, Spotify

Article Authors: Alexis Carnduff, Maddie Ulrich, Kyle Logan, Alec Zane, Matthew Hagele, MA, David Puder, MD

There are no conflicts of interest for this episode.

In Conscientiousness Part 1 we explored the sub-facets of conscientiousness, summarizing the advantages and disadvantages of each. Today, in Part 2, we will focus largely on conscientiousness in daily life, psychopathology, and therapy. 

Review of the Big Five: 

  • Extroversion: gregariousness, enthusiasm, assertiveness 

  • Neuroticism: anxiety, impulsivity, anger

  • Agreeableness: compassion, politeness, maternal tendencies

  • Oppennes: abstract ideas, moved by art and music

  • Conscientiousness contains these domains:

    • Competence: good judgement, keeps informed, makes intelligent decisions

    • Order: Likes things in place, picky about how jobs should be done

    • Dutifulness: attends work/school even when under the weather, pays debts quickly, does jobs carefully

    • Achievement striving: clear about goals, works towards goals in an orderly way, strives to achieve and be excellent at what they do

    • Self-discipline: paces oneself well to meet deadlines, possesses self discipline

    • Deliberation: rarely makes decisions in haste, always considers consequences, plans trips carefully, thinks twice before answering

Behavior is Often Driven by Personal Goals

High-conscientious individuals tend to exercise initiative in setting and achieving goals, and make sacrifices in order to achieve those goals. 

A 26-week study of salespeople found that conscientiousness is correlated with sales volume (r = .21), and a correlation between a salesman’s conscientiousness and supervisor’s satisfaction of job performance (r = .29). Assumptions about conscientiousness and goal setting were also observed with conscientious people having more commitment to accomplishing their goals (r = .35) (Barrick et al.1993).

Table displaying the intercorrelation matrix between variables such as general mental ability, conscientiousness, emotional stability, and job performance. Includinig correlation values, alpha estimates, and supervisory ratings of job performance.

Table 1. Intercorrelation Matrix” Barrick, et al. (1993)

A different study looked at the connection between orderliness (a facet of conscientiousness), disgust, and political beliefs. The authors found a small correlation between orderliness and political conservatism of r=.13 but did not find an association between industriousness and political orientation. The authors also found a correlation between orderliness and disgust (r=.22) and went into detail about possible evolutionary mechanisms for this correlation (Xu, X. et al. 2019).

Diagram illustrating the relationships between DS-R overall trait disgust, orderliness, industriousness, and the composite political orientation (PO) score.

Figure 2a. Relationships among Orderliness, Industriousness, trait disgust, and the composite political orientation (PO) score” (Xu, X., et al. 2019)

Advantages and Disadvantages of High Conscientiousness

Advantages

There are many reasons why high levels of conscientiousness can be beneficial for an individual. Higher levels of conscientiousness are associated with less arrests, better BMI, increased longevity and much more. Given the associations with many significant life outcomes, is it important to understand how individuals with high conscientiousness obtain a healthy, wealthy, and happy life. These associations may also provide insight on the progression of conscientiousness across the lifespan. Conscientious individuals choose positive, foundation-building behaviors within particular domains, like exercising and studying, which often corresponds with denying short-term, enjoyable gains, like relaxing in front of the TV or going out with friends. One study viewed this as a tendency to “invest” in one’s future, similar to how someone would invest money instead of letting it burn a hole in their pocket (Hill and Jackson, 2016). According to the study, the “invest and accrue” model of conscientiousness is a way of looking at a high conscientious person’s lifespan and explaining how individual choices a person makes in the present will affect the trajectory of their future in the long run. People higher in conscientiousness are more forward-thinking. They are willing to ‘suffer’ and give up things temporarily in the present for long term rewards in the future. 

Higher conscientiousness is also associated with higher empathy. A study found a correlation of .445 of conscientiousness and empathy which was only exceeded by agreeableness with a correlation of .532 (Melchers, et al. 2016).

Disadvantages of conscientiousness 

Risk of perfectionism and workaholism

One interesting study showed greater drops in life satisfaction following unemployment for those higher in conscientiousness, with a correlation between 0.2 and 0.3. There means there is a larger drop in well-being for high conscientiousness people following a job termination. If you’re high in conscientiousness, you might be very impacted by the feeling from one particular difficult exam or failure.

If you end up being a therapist for someone with high conscientiousness, and you are not high in conscientiousness, it is important to try and understand why it would be so devastating for them to lose their job. Empathy towards that person may sound like, “you value work and moving forward in your career. It would be so devastating to lose that ability even for this period of time. I can see why you would get down on yourself.” If you are not as high in conscientiousness you might not understand just how important career is going to be to your patient for their happiness. People with different levels of conscientiousness will have different levels of happiness that come from career. This difference in life satisfaction was pronounced for people 1 standard deviation above the mean (Boyce 2010). 

The graph compares individuals with high (+1 SD) and low (-1 SD) conscientiousness, illustrating a larger drop in life satisfaction for those with higher conscientiousness.
The table includes independent variables such as conscientiousness, years unemployed, and interaction terms, with coefficients (b), standard errors (SE), beta values (β), and d-scores for two regression models.

Based on a study in 2016, people who are very high in conscientiousness fare poorly under conditions of high stress (Carter 2016). The study showed that high people with very high conscientiousness had more obsessive-compulsive behavior compared to their moderate peers.  For those unfamiliar with the diagnoses, Obsessive Compulsive Personality Disorder is not the same as Obsessive Compulsive Disorder. OCD is a disorder where one has obsessive thoughts (like being contaminated by touching a door handle) that usually produce a compulsion to do something (washing their hands multiple times). OCPD is conscientiousness taken to the extreme. This individual may not be able to get started in the morning unless all of their to-do lists are organized. It’s the extreme of orderliness to the point that it actually gets in the way of accomplishing goals. “Taken to the extreme: (a) competence can be conceptualized as perfectionism, a pathological striving toward perfection; (b) orderliness as fastidiousness, an obsession with cleanliness and order; (c) dutifulness as punctiliousness, an obsession with attaining moral behavioral standards; (d) achievement motivation as workaholism, an obsession with achievement and perfection in work-related tasks; (e) self-discipline as doggedness, evinced by single-minded, rigid determination; and (f) cautiousness as ruminative deliberation, a tendency to overthink and stall action to avoid unwanted or uncertain outcomes” (Carter, 2016, p.511)

Table and graphs showing model-data fit for linear and curvilinear models predicting obsessive-compulsive tendencies from conscientiousness facets, including dutifulness, self-discipline, and cautiousness.
Table displaying path coefficients for linear and curvilinear models by conscientiousness facet (competence, orderliness, self-discipline) and well-being outcomes (job satisfaction, life satisfaction, negative affect, positive affect, work stress).

Conscientiousness and emotions

One study looked at the relationship between conscientiousness and emotions. They found that conscientiousness had a significant relationship with general negative affect (population correlation of ρ = −.33), most basic emotions (median correlation of (.22), proneness to guilt (.21), and the experience of guilt (−.32) (Fayard, 2012).



Conscientiousness in Daily Life

What does a highly conscientious person look like? How do they interact with those around them? What does low conscientiousness look like in practical terms? It is helpful to recognize these common signs in order to better relate to patients and to people in general. 

High conscientiousness

There are certain behaviors that are associated with conscientiousness. A study in 2009 scientifically categorized ten of these behaviors (Hirsh, 2009)

Conscientiousness associations: 

  • Discussed sexual matters with a male friend (−.23)

    • They would probably be less likely to talk about sexual matters with their therapist or psychiatrist, even if it’s currently happening. This is why we need to bring it up as mental health professionals.

  • Lounged around my house without any clothes on (−.22)

  • Picked up a hitch-hiker (−.21)

  • Read a tabloid paper (−.19)

  • Drove or rode in a car without a seatbelt (−.19)

  • Swore around other people (−.18)

  • Spent an hour at a time daydreaming (−.18)

  • Shopped at a second-hand thrift shop (−.18)

  • Told a dirty joke (−.18)

  • Listened to music (+.18)

Another study found people with high conscientiousness having cortical thickening of the prefrontal cortex. This is the same part of the brain that deals with delay of gratification (Riccelli, 2017).

Brain scans showing positive correlation with cortical thickness (A) and negative correlations with surface area (B), cortical volume (C), and local gyrification index (D) in various brain regions.

We can change our brain. We know that this is not purely genetic and a lot of it is our environment (parenting and choices that are made change the brain over time). Studies show that we can make choices to strengthen certain areas of our brain through meditation and strength training. Setting a budget and practicing discipline changes the way our brain is wired over time. We can also rewire how empathic we are by practicing empathic stances.

Low conscientiousness

People lower in conscientiousness may seem disorganized and tend to be unreliable and careless. They are procrastinators, less driven to succeed, and exhibit less goal-oriented behaviors. According to Dewitte & Schouwenburg (2002), the most typical symptom of procrastination is underperformance, which is caused by not having enough time to do one’s best work. Conscientiousness seems to explain most of the variation in procrastination items, regardless of the metric used. Conscientiousness and procrastination have a -.69 correlation. In general, people who are high conscientious are not procrastinators (Dewitte & Schouwenburg, 2002)

Table showing intercorrelations between personality domains (extraversion, neuroticism, conscientiousness, openness) and aspects of impulsivity (premeditation, urgency, sensation seeking, perseverance, procrastination).

Table 1. The intercorrelations (decimals omitted) of the input, intermediate, and criterion variables of the proposed linear model (phase 1)” (Dewitte & Schouwenburg, 2002)

Career Success 

In Judge (1999) the correlation between conscientiousness and job satisfaction was the most consistent result in this study (0.42), or about 18% variance.

Table showing the relationship between Big Five traits (neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeableness, conscientiousness), general mental ability, and intrinsic career success.

Longer Lifespans 

The Terman Life-City Study of children Friedman (1993) showed: “The magnitudes of the effects for low conscientiousness and low cheerfulness (relative hazards between 1.2 and 1.3) are comparable to other known risk factors for mortality (systolic blood pressure and serum cholesterol, have been found to have relative hazards of approximately 1.3 and 1.2)” (Friedman et al. 1993).

What’s even more important is a person’s social support, including marriage quality, contact with friends and family, and group membership. If you have poor relationships, the hazard ratio ranges from 1.5-2. Thinking about this in the context of current suicidality (at least one suicide thought in the last month), which is 10%, a recent CDC study asked unpaid caregivers if they had one suicidal thought within the last month and 30% said “yes.” This high hazard ratio, with the lack of quality relationships and difficulty forming them, leads to an increased risk of mortality.

Conscientiousness and Subjective Well-Being (SWB)

A 1998 study looked at the relationship between personality traits and SWB, and found a 0.22 correlation between SWB and conscientiousness (DeNeve and Cooper, 1998).

Table showing overall correlations and contrasts between subjective well-being (SWB) conceptualizations (life satisfaction, happiness, positive affect, negative affect) and Big Five personality traits.

Driving Safety 

Adult differences in conscientiousness, and agreeableness, may reflect internalized tendencies in regulating anger and frustration. Presumably, conscientious people are better at resisting temptation (e.g. speeding) and controlling anger and negative affect (e.g. hostile reactions to being cut off in traffic). In a meta-analysis of vehicular accident research, Arthur, Barrett, and Alexander (1991) identified four categories of variables: personality, cognitive ability, information-processing, and demographic variables. 

    • Methods used: Goldberg's 100 Unipolar Markers, NEO-FFI, and Driving Behavior Questionnaire (DBQ)

    • Results: multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was used to test for differences between the accident and no-accident groups on the five personality factors. Results of the overall MANOVA, using the Hotelling-Lawley Trace statistic, were significant, F(5, 221) — 2.43, p < .05. 

Table comparing Big Five personality traits between at-fault accident and no-accident college student groups. The table includes means, standard deviations (SD), and t-values, with significant differences in conscientiousness.

The results of this study suggest that individuals who describe themselves as self-disciplined, responsible, reliable, and dependable are less likely to be involved in driving accidents than those who describe themselves as lower on these attributes.

Conscientiousness and Psychopathology

Conscientiousness has shown consistent and robust associations with mental disorders across the lifespan. It also has moderate to strong associations with all major forms of psychopathology.

Axis 1 disorders

Low conscientiousness has very large effect sizes (Cohen’s d>1.0) with Axis 1 disorders (Kotov, et al. 2010). This means that people who were lower in conscientiousness had an increase in depression, anxiety, OCD, and substance use disorders. This came from a quantitative review of 66 meta-analyses and 175 studies published from 1980 to 2007 reviewing associations between the Big Five personality types and depression, anxiety, and substance use disorders (SUD).

The table displays effect sizes (d) for personality traits such as neuroticism, extraversion, disinhibition, conscientiousness, agreeableness, and openness, with 80% credibility intervals (CrI).

Table 4. Average Effect Sizes Corrected for Unreliability of Personality Scales” (Kotov, et al. 2010)

Common mental illnesses were strongly connected with personality (some effect sizes with Cohen's d > 2.0 when examining neuroticism and mental illness). All disorders were found to have a positive relationship with high neuroticism, low conscientiousness, and low extraversion (Kotov, et al. 2010, p.808).

Axis 2 disorders

A meta-analysis examined the relationships between each trait of the Five-Factor model and each of the 10 DSM-IV personality diagnostic categories. They included paranoid, schizoid, schizotypal, antisocial, borderline, histrionic, narcissistic, avoidant, dependent, and obsessive-compulsive categories. 12 studies published from 1990 to 1998 met the inclusion criteria. Small to medium effect sizes were found for axis II disorders (Saulsman & Page, 2004).

Table 5. Sample size weighted mean effect size estimates for each DSM-IV personality disorder and five-factor model personality dimension combination” (Saulsman &amp; Page, 2004)

Table 5. Sample size weighted mean effect size estimates for each DSM-IV personality disorder and five-factor model personality dimension combination” (Saulsman & Page, 2004)

Disorders that are characterized by emotional distress had positive associations with neuroticism (paranoid (d=.28, P<.01), schizotypal (d= .36, P<.01), borderline (d= .49, P <.01), avoidant (d=.48, P <.01), and dependent (d=.41, P<.01). Neuroticism and agreeableness were the most prominent and consistent with their effect sizes across all the disorders. Disorders characterized by gregariousness (histrionic and narcissistic) had positive associations with extraversion. Disorders with interpersonal difficulties (paranoid, schizotypal, antisocial, borderline, and narcissistic) had negative associations with agreeableness. Disorders with orderliness (obsessive-compulsive) had positive associations with conscientiousness, while disorders with recklessness (borderline and antisocial) had negative associations with conscientiousness (Saulsman & Page, 2004).

Substance abuse

A study from 2004 gives insight into the relationship between conscientiousness and substance abuse. Impulsive people tend to be low in conscientiousness and association has been seen with impulsivity and heavy substance use. “There seems to be a linear relationship between substance use and both conscientiousness and openness, as abstainers score comparatively high on conscientiousness and low on openness, unlike heavy users” (Walton, 2004, p.518).

Participants in this study were undergraduate students at a large Midwestern university enrolled in an introductory psychology course. They directly compared heavy users to abstainers and moderate users. “The findings were consistent with the argument that there is a linear relationship between substance use and psychological distress. Heavy users scored lower than abstainers on measures of conscientiousness t(72)= 4.18, p<.001, and its constituent facets of cautiousness, t(72)= 4.52, p<.001, dutifulness, t(72)= 4.53, p<.001, and perfectionism, t(71)= 2.47, p<.05. Compared to moderate users, heavy users scored lower on conscientiousness than moderate users, t(72)= 2.57, p<.05, cautiousness, t(72)= 2.96, p<.01, dutifulness, t(72)=2.49, p<.05, and perfectionism, t(72)=2.15, p<.05. Heavy users scored higher than abstainers on Barratt’s Impulsivity Scale (BIS), t(70)= 3.46, p<.01” (Walton, 2004, p.522)

Table showing descriptive statistics, F tests, and regression coefficients for personality traits (extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, etc.) across categories of alcohol use (abstainers, moderate users, heavy users).

Table 1. Descriptive statistics, F tests, and regression coefficients for categories of alcohol use” (Walton, 2004)

Antisocial and criminal behavior

Low conscientiousness has been associated with antisocial behavior, lack of constraint, behavior problems in adolescence, and substance abuse (Ozer, 2006).

Links to ADHD

Out of the Big Five personality domains, conscientiousness had the largest correlation with attention problems, with participants scoring lower in conscientiousness having more attention problems. The correlation of -.58 was seen with self-reported conscientiousness and -.40 for spouse-reported conscientiousness (Nigg, 2002).

Table showing mean correlations between Wender-Stein ADHD scales and Big Five personality traits (extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism, openness) based on self and spouse ratings across various samples.

Table 2. Mean Correlations Between Wender-Stein ADHD Scales and Big Five Self- and Spouse Ratings Across All Six Samples (Weighted by Sample Size)” (Nigg, 2002)

High and Low Conscientiousness in Therapy

Working with a high conscientiousness patient 

  • Respect the patient’s time and schedule.

  • Communicate thoughts directly. 

  • Share appreciation for the patient’s considerate and hard-working attitude. 

  • Therapy works better because patients are likely to make an effort, tolerate discomfort, and delay impulses and desires.

  • A key problem of these patients may be being overworked.

  • Those with higher conscientiousness are more likely to attend their therapy sessions and take their medications as prescribed. 

Working with a low conscientiousness patient

  • These patients are more likely to become discouraged or frustrated with therapy that requires hard work.

  • These patients may be less likely to be successful in their career but they are not less likely to desire success. 

  • CBT may be more difficult due to a lack of engagement with homework 

  • Those with lower conscientiousness may need help setting up structures for taking their medications, and plans of action if doses are missed to make up for inconsistency. 

  • “CRT could be used to enhance cognitive functions, such as executive function, that subserves conscientiousness” (Javaras, 2019, p.6)

  • Patients may appear resistant to therapy.

  • Roll with resistance without being judgmental if they don’t get it right away.

  • Have realistic but hopeful expectations. In this study of 119 patients over 2 years “conscientiousness was significantly correlated with a good outcome, r = .35” (Miller, 1991, p. 431). But, this is not a death sentence to therapy failing.   

  • Consider a program with more structure in order to keep them engaged

    • Understand that the beginning will be difficult, but forming positive relationships with others in groups will be helpful for them.

    • Group therapy may be a good option for people low in conscientiousness. Once they meet friends and begin to feel like they’re part of the group, it can carry them through a lot of their treatment. They begin to get the benefits of therapy in a controlled environment. Get them to stay in the program for the first week or two, and they will usually stay plugged in until the end. The question then is whether or not their conscientiousness improves over time.

  • Find their underlying driving forces, and get them to talk about what they actually care about over and over again (motivational interviewing). 

  • Through behavioral therapy, they can make changes to their conscientiousness score, for example making small steps like being on time for meetings (McAdams, et al. 2018)

  • It’s hard to get people with lower conscientiousness to exercise. You may need to access other pleasure centers that are not as structured, such as surfing or getting together with friends for some physical activity. It can be fun at times to work with someone who is very motivated, but there are people that are lower in conscientiousness who desire change, and I’ll help them access a driving force based on what is already a natural drive in their lives. 

References

Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y., Whitehouse, W. G., & Hogan, M. E. (2006). Prospective 

incidence of first onsets and recurrences of depression in individuals at high and low cognitive risk for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 145–156. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16492105 

Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y., Whitehouse, W. G., Hogan, M. E., Tashman, N. A., Steinberg, 

D. L., ... & Donovan, P. (1999). Depressogenic cognitive styles: Predictive validity, information processing and personality characteristics, and developmental origins. Behaviour research and therapy, 37(6), 503-531. 

Alloy, L. B., Whitehouse, W. G., Lapkin, J. B., Abramson, L. Y., Hogan, M. E., Rose, D. T. et al. 

(2000). The Temple-Wisconsin Cognitive Vulnerability to Depression (CVD) Project: Lifetime history of Axis 1 psychopathology in individuals at high and low cognitive risk for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 403–418. 

Altemeyer, B. (2006). The authoritarians. Winnipeg, Canada: University of Manitoba.

Amin, N., Schuur, M., Gusareva, E. S., Isaacs, A., Aulchenko, Y. S., Kirichenko, A. V., ... & van 

Duijn, C. M. (2012). A genome-wide linkage study of individuals with high scores on NEO personality traits. Molecular psychiatry, 17(10), 1031-1041. https://www.nature.com/articles/mp201197.pdf?origin=ppub 

Arthur, W., Barret, G. V., & Alexander, R. A. (1991). Prediction of vehicular accident involvement: A meta-analysis. Human performance, 4(2), 89-105. 

Arthur Jr, W., & Graziano, W. G. (1996). The five‐factor model, conscientiousness, and driving accident involvement. Journal of personality, 64(3), 593-618. 

Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K., & Strauss, J. P. (1993). Conscientiousness and 

performance of sales representatives: Test of the mediating effects of goal setting. Journal of applied psychology, 78(5), 715.

Barrick M.R. , Mount M.K. (1991). The Big Five personality dimensions and job performance: 

a meta-analysis. Personnel psychology, 44(1), 1–26

Bergeman, C. S., Chlpuer, H. M., Plomin, R., Pedersen, N. L., McClearn, G. E., Nesselroade, 

J. R., ... & McCrae, R. R. (1993). Genetic and environmental effects on openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness: An adoption/twin study. Journal of personality, 61(2), 159-179.

Borman, W. C., White, L. A., Pulakos, E. D., & Oppler, S. H. (1991). Models of supervisory 

job performance ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76(6), 863.

Boyce, C. J., Wood, A. M., & Brown, G. D. (2010). The dark side of conscientiousness: 

Conscientious people experience greater drops in life satisfaction following unemployment. Journal of Research in Personality, 44(4), 535-539.

Brandt, N. D., Mike, A., & Jackson, J. J. (2019). Do school-related experiences impact 

personality? Selection and socialization effects of impulse control. Developmental psychology, 55(12), 2561.

Brummett, B. H., Siegler, I. C., Day, R. S., & Costa, P. T. (2008). Personality as a Predictor of 

Dietary Quality in Spouses During Midlife.

Carter, N.T., Dalal, D.K., Boyce, A., O'Connell, M.S., Kung, M‐C., & Delgado, K. (2014). 

Uncovering curvilinear relationships between conscientiousness and job performance: How theoretically appropriate measurement makes an empirical difference. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99, 564– 586.

Carter, N. T., Guan, L., Maples, J. L., Williamson, R. L., & Miller, J. D. (2016). The 

downsides of extreme conscientiousness for psychological well‐being: The role of obsessive compulsive tendencies. Journal of personality, 84(4), 510-522.

Cianci, A.M., Klein, H.J., & Seijts, G.H. (2010). The effect of negative feedback on tension and 

subsequent performance: The main and interactive effects of goal content and conscientiousness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 618– 630.

Costa Jr, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (2008). The Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R)

Sage Publications, Inc.

Costa Jr, P. T., McCrae, R. R., & Dye, D. A. (1991). Facet scales for agreeableness and 

conscientiousness: A revision of the NEO Personality Inventory. Personality and individual Differences, 12(9), 887-898. 

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow. The Psychology of Optimal Experience. New York 

(HarperPerennial) 1990.

de Moor, M., Costa, P., Terracciano, A. et al. Meta-analysis of genome-wide association 

studies for personality. Mol Psychiatry 17, 337–349 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2010.128

DeNeve, K. M., & Cooper, H. (1998). The happy personality: A meta-analysis of 137 personality 

traits and subjective well-being. Psychological bulletin, 124(2), 197.

Denissen, J. J. A., Bleidorn, W., Hennecke, M., Luhmann, M., Orth, U., Specht, J., & 

Zimmermann, J. (2017). Uncovering the Power of Personality to Shape Income. Psychological Science, 95679761772443.

Dewitte, S., & Schouwenburg, H. C. (2002). Procrastination, temptations, and incentives: 

The struggle between the present and the future in procrastinators and the punctual. European Journal of personality, 16(6), 469-489.

DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between facets and domains: 10 aspects 

of the Big Five. Journal of personality and social psychology, 93(5), 880.

DeYoung C.G. (2010). Personality neuroscience and the biology of traits. Social and 

Personality Psychology Compass, 4(12), 1165–80.

Digman JM. Personality structure: emergence of the five-factor model. Ann Rev Psychol. 

1990;41:417–440.

Dima D. , Friston K.J., Stephan K.E., Frangou S. (2015). Neuroticism and conscientiousness 

respectively constrain and facilitate short-term plasticity within the working memory neural network. Human Brain Mapping, 36(10), 4158–63.

Duckitt, J., Bizumic, B., Krauss, S. W., & Heled, E. (2010). A tripartite approach to right-wing 

authoritarianism: The authoritarianism-conservatism-traditionalism model. Political Psychology, 31, 685–715.

Duncan J. (2013). The structure of cognition: attentional episodes in mind and brain. Neuron, 

80(1), 35–50.

Eisenberg, N., Duckworth, A. L., Spinrad, T. L., & Valiente, C. (2014). Conscientiousness: 

Origins in childhood?. Developmental psychology, 50(5), 1331.

Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Ellsworth, P. (1972). Emotion in the human face: Guidelines for 

research and an integration of findings. New York: Pergamon Press.

Fayard, J. V., Roberts, B. W., Robins, R. W., & Watson, D. (2012). Uncovering the 

affective core of conscientiousness: The role of self‐conscious emotions. Journal of Personality, 80(1), 1-32.

Forbes C.E. , Poore J.C., Krueger F., Barbey A.K., Solomon J., Grafman J. (2014). The role of 

executive function and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the expression of neuroticism and conscientiousness. Social Neuroscience, 9(2), 139–51.

Friedman, H. S., Tucker, J. S., Tomlinson-Keasey, C., Schwartz, J. E., Wingard, D. L., & 

Criqui, M. H. (1993). Does childhood personality predict longevity?. Journal of personality and social psychology, 65(1), 176.

Friedman H.S. (2000). Long-term relations of personality and health: dynamisms, 

mechanisms, tropisms. Journal of Personality, 68(6), 1089–107.

Ge X, Conger RD. 1999. Adjustment problems and emerging personality characteristics from 

early to late adolescence. Am. J. Community Psychol. 27:429–59

Goldberg, L. R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality traits. American 

psychologist, 48(1), 26.

Goldberg, L. R. (1999). A broad-bandwidth, public-domain, personality inventory measuring 

the lower-level facets of several five-factor models. In I. Mervielde, I. Deary, F. De Fruyt, & F. Ostendorf (Eds.), Personality psychology in Europe (Vol. 7; pp. 7–28). Tilburg, The Netherlands: Tilburg University Press.

Haidt, J., McCauley, C., & Rozin, P. (1994). Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A 

scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 701–713.

Hill, P. L., & Jackson, J. J. (2016). The invest-and-accrue model of conscientiousness. 

Review of General Psychology, 20(2), 141-154.

Hirsh, J. B., DeYoung, C. G., & Peterson, J. B. (2009). Metatraits of the Big Five 

differentially predict engagement and restraint of behavior. Journal of personality, 77(4), 1085-1102.

Hogan, R., Mankin, D., Conway, J., & Fox, S. (1970). Personality correlates of undergraduate 

marijuana use. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 35(1p1), 58.

Jang, K. L., Livesley, W. J., & Vemon, P. A. (1996). Heritability of the big five personality 

dimensions and their facets: a twin study. Journal of personality, 64(3), 577-592.

Johnson, A. M., Vernon, P. A., & Feiler, A. R. (2008). Behavioral genetic studies of personality: An 

introduction and review of the results of 50 years of research. In G. J. Boyle, G. Matthews, & D. H. Saklofske (Eds.), The Sage handbook of personality theory and assessment. Vol. 1: Personality theories and models (pp. 145–173). London, England: Sage.

Jones, M. C. (1968). Personality correlates and antecedents of drinking patterns in adult 

males. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 32(1), 2.

Judge, T. A., Higgins, C. A., Thoresen, C. J., & Barrick, M. R. (1999). The big five personality 

traits, general mental ability, and career success across the life span. Personnel psychology, 52(3), 621-652.

Kapogiannis D. , Sutin A., Davatzikos C., Costa P.Jr, Resnick S. (2012). The five factors of 

personality and regional cortical variability in the baltimore longitudinal study of aging. Human Brain Mapping, 34(11), 2829–40.

Kendler, K. S., & Myers, J. (2009). The genetic and environmental relationship between major 

depression and the five‐factor model of personality. Psychological Medicine, 40. doi:10.1017/S0033291709991140

Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: 


continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental psychology, 36(2), 220.

Kotov, R., Gamez, W., Schmidt, F. L., & Watson, D. (2010). Linking “big” personality traits to 

anxiety, depressive and substance use disorders: A meta‐analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 768– 821.

Labouvie, E. W., & McGee, C. R. (1986). Relation of personality to alcohol and drug use in 

adolescence. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology, 54(3), 289.

McAdams, D. P., Shiner, R. L., & Tackett, J. L. (Eds.). (2018). Handbook of personality 

development. Guilford Publications.

McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1987). Validation of the five-factor model of personality 

across instruments and observers. Journal of personality and social psychology, 52(1), 81.

McCrae, R. R., & Costa Jr, P. T. (1988). Recalled parent‐child relations and adult personality. 

Journal of Personality, 56(2), 417-434.

McCrae, R. R., and Costa, P. T. (1985). Comparison of EPI and psychoticism scales with 

measures of the five-factor model of personality. Pers. Individ. Differ. 6, 587–597. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(85)90008-X

Melchers, M. C., Li, M., Haas, B. W., Reuter, M., Bischoff, L., & Montag, C. (2016). 

Similar personality patterns are associated with empathy in four different countries. Frontiers in psychology, 7, 290.

Miller, T. R. (1991). The Psychotherapeutic Utility of the Five-Factor Model of Personality: A Clinician’s Experience. Journal of Personality Assessment, 57(3), 415–433. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5703_3

Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. I. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. 

Science, 244(4907), 933-938.

Mongrain, M., & Blackburn, S. (2005). Cognitive vulnerability, lifetime risk, and the 

recurrence of major depression in graduate students. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29, 747–768.

Murphy, K. R., & Lee, S. L. (1993, April). Does conscientiousness explain the relationship 

between integrity and job performance? Paper presented at the 8th annual conference of the Society of Industrial and Organizational Psychology, San Francisco

Murphy K.R. , Lee S.L. (1994). Personality variables related to integrity test scores: the role of 

conscientiousness. Journal of Business and Psychology, 8(4), 413–24.

Nigg, J. T., John, O. P., Blaskey, L. G., Huang-Pollock, C. L., Willcutt, E. G., Hinshaw, S. 

P., & Pennington, B. (2002). Big five dimensions and ADHD symptoms: links between personality traits and clinical symptoms. Journal of personality and social psychology, 83(2), 451.

Ozer, D. J., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2006). Personality and the prediction of consequential 

outcomes. Annu. Rev. Psychol., 57, 401-421.

Poropat, A. E. (2009). A meta-analysis of the five-factor model of personality and academic 

performance. Psychological bulletin, 135(2), 322.

Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M., & Malle, B. F. (1994). Social dominance orientation: A 

personality variable predicting social and political attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 741–763.

Ralph, J. A., & Mineka, S. (1998). Attributional style and self-esteem: The prediction of 

emotional distress following a midterm exam. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 203–215.response style. Pers. Individ. Differ. 17, 303–311. Doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(94)90278-X

Richendoller, N. R., and Weaver, J. B. (1994). Exploring the links between personality and empathic 

Roberta Riccelli, Nicola Toschi, Salvatore Nigro, Antonio Terracciano, Luca Passamonti, 

Surface-based morphometry reveals the neuroanatomical basis of the five-factor model of personality, Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, Volume 12, Issue 4, April 2017, Pages 671–684. 

Roberts, B.W., Jackson, J.J., Fayard, J.V., Edmonds, G., & Meints, J. (2009). 

Conscientiousness. In M.R. Leary & R.H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences in social behavior (pp. 369– 381). New York: Guilford.

Roberts, B.W.; Jackson, J.J.; Fayard, J.V.; Edmonds, G.; Meints, J (2009). "Chapter 25. 

Conscientiousness". In Mark R. Leary, & Rick H. Hoyle (ed.). Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior. New York/London: The Guildford Press. pp. 257–273. ISBN 978-1-59385-647-2.

Roberts, J.S., Donoghue, J.R., & Laughlin, J.E. (2000). A general item response theory model 

for unfolding unidimensional polytomous responses. Applied Psychological Measurement, 24, 3– 32.

Rodrigo A.H. , Di Domenico S.I., Graves B., et al. . (2016). Linking trait-based phenotypes to 

prefrontal cortex activation during inhibitory control. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(1), 55–65.

Samejima, F. (1969). Estimation of a latent ability using a response pattern of graded scores. 

Psychometrika Monographs, 34(Suppl. 4), 1– 100.

Sanchez‐Roige, S., Gray, J. C., MacKillop, J., Chen, C. H., & Palmer, A. A. (2018). The genetics 

of human personality. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 17(3), e12439.

Schouwenburg, H. C., & Groenewoud, T. (2001). Study motivation under social 

temptation: effects of trait procrastination. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 229–240.

Shedler, J., & Block, J. (1990). Adolescent drug use and psychological health: A longitudinal 

inquiry. American psychologist, 45(5), 612.

Shiner R, Masten AS, Tellegen A. 2002. A developmental perspective on personality in 

emerging adulthood: childhood antecedents and concurrent adaptation. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 83:1165–77

Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., & Peake, P. K. (1990). Predicting adolescent cognitive and self-regulatory 

competencies from preschool delay of gratification: Identifying diagnostic conditions. Developmental psychology, 26(6), 978.

Skinner, H. A., & Allen, B. A. (1982). Alcohol dependence syndrome: measurement and 

validation. Journal of abnormal psychology, 91(3), 199.

Stillman, T. F., Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., Lambert, N. M., Fincham, F. D., & Brewer, L. E. 

(2010). Personal philosophy and personnel achievement: Belief in free will predicts better job performance. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 1(1), 43-50.

Tangney, J. P., & Dearing, R. L. (2002). Shame and guilt. New York: Guilford Press.

Trull TJ, Sher KJ. 1994. Relationship between the Five Factor model of personality and Axis I 

disorders in a nonclinical sample. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 103:350–60

Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., Kurzban, R., & DeScioli, P. (2013). Disgust: Evolved function and 

structure. Psychological Review, 120(1), 65–84.

van den Berg, S. M., de Moor, M. H., McGue, M., Pettersson, E., Terracciano, A., Verweij, K. J.,... 

Boomsma, D. I. (2014). Harmonization of neuroticism and Extraversion phenotypes across inventories and cohorts in the Genetics of Personality Consortium: An application of the item response theory. Behavior Genetics, 44, 295–313.

Verona E, Patrick CJ, Joiner TE. 2001. Psychopathy, antisocial personality, and suicide risk. J. 

Abnorm. Psychol. 110:462–70

Vukasović, T., & Bratko, D. (2015). Heritability of personality: a meta-analysis of behavior genetic 

studies. Psychological bulletin, 141(4), 769.

Walton, K. E., & Roberts, B. W. (2004). On the relationship between substance use and 

personality traits: Abstainers are not maladjusted. Journal of Research in Personality, 38(6), 515-535.

Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1992). On traits and temperament: General and specific factors of 

emotional experience and their relation to the five‐factor model. Journal of Personality, 60, 441– 476.

Watson, D., Hubbard, B., & Wiese, D. (2000). Self‐other agreement in personality and 

affectivity: The role of acquaintanceship, trait visibility, and assumed similarity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 546– 558.

Wiebe RP. 2004. Delinquent behavior and the Five Factor model: hiding in the adaptive 

landscape? Individ. Differ. Res. 2:38–62

Xu, X., Karinen, A. K., Chapman, H. A., Peterson, J. B., & Plaks, J. E. (2019). An orderly 

personality partially explains the link between trait disgust and political conservatism. Cognition and Emotion

Yeager, D. S., & Dweck, C. S. (2012). Mindsets That Promote Resilience: When Students Believe That Personal Characteristics Can Be Developed. Educational Psychologist, 47(4), 302–314. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2012.722805

Previous
Previous

Episode 100: The Big Five: Agreeableness

Next
Next

Episode 098: The Big Five: Openness